@47th ECVP 2025 Mainz
August 24, 2025
Evidence Accumulation Models assume that, upon stimulus presentation, the decision maker:
Neural signals reflecting sensory or internal information relevant to a choice.
“Are dots moving to the right or to the left?”
Neurons in LIP, dlPFC, or striatum ramp up/down over time.
Reflects accumulated evidence
Cognitive Term | Neural Interpretation |
---|---|
Evidence | Sensory neuron firing rates |
Accumulation | Integration in parietal/frontal areas |
Noise | Trial-to-trial neural variability |
Bound | Decision threshold in firing/activity |
Total Response Time (RT) is modeled as the sum of three sequential stages:
Stages (1) and (3) are captured in the nondecision time (Ter) parameter.
EAMs decompose decisions into:
Drift rate
Threshold
Reflects evidence strength
⬆ Drift: fast & accurate decisions
⬇ Drift: slow, error-prone
Manipulated by stimulus discriminability/task difficulty
Set before stimulus onset
Reflect response caution/cognitive control/bias/preference
⬆ Threshold: Slower but more accurate
⬇ Threshold: Faster but error-prone
Manipulate via pre-trial cues or instructions.
In relative evidence models (e.g., Wiener process, Diffusion Decision Model):
In racing accumulator models (e.g., LBA, RDM):
Each option has its own accumulator tracking absolute evidence.
Decision is made by the first accumulator to reach threshold.
Can handle multiple alternatives (not just binary choices).
The Wiener process (Brownian motion) is the foundation of many decision models (Smith & Ratcliff, 2024). It models the accumulation of evidence as a noisy process:
It is the expected distribution of the time until the process first hits or crosses one or the other boundary. This results in a bivariate distribution, over responses and hitting times.
Navarro & Fuss, 2009; Wabersich & Vandekerckhov, 2014. Copyright 2009, Joachim Vandekerckhove and Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Belgium
The Full DDM accounts for more behavioral phenomena by allowing trial-to-trial variability in key parameters:
Drift rate
Starting point
Non-decision time
: decision boundary
: starting point
: drift rate
: non-decision time
: noise scale (usually fixed to 1)
: SD of drift rate across trials
: variability in start point
: variability in non-decision time
Boehm, U., Annis, J., Frank, … & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2018). Estimating across-trial variability parameters of the Diffusion Decision Model: Expert advice and recommendations. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 87, 46-75. Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J. N. (1998). Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions. Psychological Science, 9(5), 347-356.
Adding variability improves the model’s ability to:
However, it increases computational demands.
Instead of a single process choosing between boundaries, the RDM uses multiple independent diffusion processes, one per option. Each accumulator races toward its threshold. The first to cross wins. Tillman, Van Zandt, & Logan, 2020
Model | Core Mechanism | Key Strengths |
---|---|---|
Wiener | Noisy accumulation | Simple FPT, binary outcomes |
Full DDM | Accumulation + param variability | Realistic RTs, error patterns |
Racing DM | Multiple accumulators | Handles multi-alternative decisions |
Model parameters are fixed within a trial
Evidence accumulation:
Decision thresholds:
Parameters are constant across same-type trials
Assumes:
Data should reflect evidence accumulation!
Avoid:
Random guessing
Fast guesses
Attention lapses or missing responses
Clean data = better model fit and interpretability.